Psychiatry Research
Volume 112, Issue 1,
15 September 2002
, Pages 77-81
Author links open overlay panel, ,
Abstract
Lists of phonologically similar items are more often recalled in the wrong order than phonologically dissimilar items. At recall, patients with schizophrenia were neither especially susceptible to confusing phonologically similar items nor to making disproportionate movement (i.e. order) errors with phonologically similar lists of items. We conclude that patients with schizophrenia employ recall strategies for phonologically similar items in short-term memory that are equivalent to those of healthy controls.
Introduction
The key components in short-term memory (STM) have been described by many models. Baddeley's (1986) influential working memory model includes a central executive (the controlling mechanism) with two subsidiary ‘slave’ components, specialized in processing verbally coded information (phonological loop) and visual and/or spatial information (visuospatial sketchpad). We focus on the phonological loop, assumed to comprise a phonological store capable of holding speech-based information, and an articulatory control process, which is based on inner speech. Memory traces in the phonological store decay within seconds, but can be refreshed by ‘rehearsing’ the trace into the articulatory control process, which in turn sends the memory trace back to the phonological store. This process is assumed to underlie subvocal rehearsal. Written material may be converted into a phonological code and registered in the phonological store, through the articulatory control process. Much evidence suggests that the representations in most verbal STM tasks are speech-based. Items pronounced similarly (B, D, P or cat, rat, mat) are harder to recall in serial order than items pronounced differently (C, F, J or man, egg, boat). Even though the items themselves are more likely to be recalled when similar, the order of similar sounding material is more likely to be wrong than that of the order of dissimilar sounding material (e.g. Conrad and Hull, 1964, Wickelgren, 1965). This phonological similarity effect probably arises because the store is based on a phonological code (i.e. sound or articulatory characteristics). Thus, similar sounding items will have similar phonological codes, and since accurate recall of order requires discriminating among memory traces, similar ones will be harder to discriminate. This will lead to poorer recall of the order of phonologically similar items, than if the traces were dissimilar.
An attraction of the phonological loop model is that it offers simple mechanistic accounts of certain STM deficits in patients (e.g., dementia—Belleville et al., 1996). Schizophrenic patients have been reliably shown to have problems in the STM domain (e.g., Conklin et al., 2000). We sought an explanation for verbal STM reduction (i.e., span) in terms of differences in mnemonic strategy, specifically concerning the phonological loop. First, we asked whether patients display a disproportionate phonological similarity effect. Interest in this topic in schizophrenia has ranged from examination of the phonological loop in relation to working memory in general (e.g., Salamé et al., 1998) or in relation to symptoms (e.g., hallucinations—David and Lucas, 1993; thought echo—David, 1994) to ascribing attentional/executive task (e.g., Wisconsin Card Sorting Task—Grön, 1998) performance reductions to problems in the phonological loop. Our interest was whether a disproportionate phonological similarity effect may be an important factor in patients’ poor STM (as indexed by verbal digit span), such that performance is especially worse on phonologically similar than dissimilar lists. Second, we wanted to establish whether this phonological similarity effect is driven by a disproportionate amount of movement (order) errors (e.g., A B C D E F being recalled as A B D C E F). Given that recall order of similar sounding stimuli is more likely to be wrong than that of dissimilar sounding stimuli, we predicted movement errors to provide a useful index of this process. Moreover, we expected patients to make more movement errors in the case of phonologically similar lists if employing different (i.e. inefficient) mnemonic strategies. Specifically, we hypothesized a disproportionate amount of movement errors in the phonologically similar lists, since the recalled order of phonologically confusable items is more likely to be wrong than nonconfusable items. Thus, if patients are disproportionately sensitive to the phonological similarity effect, an analysis of the order of the errors would be crucial, especially in terms of movement errors.
This study follows an earlier study that examined in detail the nature of errors in serial recall (Elvevåg et al., 2001—based on the work of Henson (1998) and Henson et al. (1996)), but did not examine the phonological similarity effect. Analysis of error patterns is useful in constraining theories concerning the nature of serial recall deficits. Our previous study concluded that patients’ limited STM span may be a result of increased forgetting during recall, and not due to specific deficits in mechanisms responsible for maintaining serial order. The current report provides a detailed analysis of data from a six-item serial recall task in which we examined phonological similarity as a function of serial position.
Section snippets
Subjects
All patients fulfilled criteria for schizophrenia or schizoaffective disorder as determined by the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV (SCID). Normal healthy control volunteers were recruited through the National Institutes of Health volunteer panel (Table 1).
Measures
Stimuli for the serial recall task were 20 lists of six single-syllable consonants, classified according to phonological similarity (phonologically similar—P, T, D, G, V, B—versus phonologically dissimilar—Q, J, M, H, R, Y). They were
Overall performance
Overall performance was scored as percentage of the total letters (i.e., 120) recalled in their correct positions as a function of phonological similarity. Lists with phonologically similar letters resulted in worse performance (mean correct: 55% for controls, 32% for patients) than those with phonologically dissimilar letters (mean correct: 85% for controls, 64% for patients). Repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVA) with group (controls, patients) as the between-group factor, and
Discussion
A meta-analysis of many studies measuring digit span found a large effect size (d=0.71—forward span) in schizophrenic patients. This was not driven by illness duration, severity of psychopathology and symptoms or medication status (Aleman et al., 1999). We compared overall percentage of errors made in the current study by controls (30%) with patients split as a function of neuroleptics (typical—56%; atypical—48%; risperidone—56%) and conclude that our findings are not simply medication
References (14)
- P. Salamé et al.
The state of functioning of working memory in schizophrenia
Schizophrenia Research
(1998)
- R.N.A. Henson
Short-term memory for serial order: the Start–End Model
Cognitive Psychology
(1998)
- G. Grön
Auditory and visual working memory performance in patients with frontal lobe damage and in schizophrenic patients with low scores on the Wisconsin Card Sorting Test
Psychiatry Research
(1998)
- S. Belleville et al.
Examination of the working memory components in normal aging and in dementia of the Alzheimer's type
Neuropsychologia
(1996)
- A. Aleman et al.
Memory impairment in schizophrenia: a meta-analysis
American Journal of Psychiatry
(1999)
- A.D. Baddeley
Working Memory
(1986)
- H.M. Conklin et al.
Verbal working memory impairment in schizophrenia patients and their first-degree relatives: evidence from the Digit-Span Task
American Journal of Psychiatry
(2000)
There are more references available in the full text version of this article.
Cited by (8)
Neurological Disorders in Schizophrenia
2009, Psychiatric Clinics of North America
Citation Excerpt :
The patient group also had impairment of phonological and semantic fluency. In the adult literature, some of the domains of verbal memory that were impaired in SZ compared with controls are difficulties recalling the first items in a list, and impaired object recognition and identification.288,289 Impairment in declarative memory, but not in nondeclarative memory, was also reported in patients with SZ.290,291
The Cognitive Neuroscience of Working Memory: Relevance to CNTRICS and Schizophrenia
2008, Biological Psychiatry
Citation Excerpt :
A number of studies have shown that individuals with schizophrenia demonstrate relatively intact performance on digit span forward tasks, particularly when the number of items is at or below working memory span (7 ± 2) (2,50,81) and when there is no verbal interference (69). Other work has shown that individuals with schizophrenia do not show disproportionate impairment for recall of lists with phonologically similar versus dissimilar items, suggesting an intact ability to represent phonological information (82). Further, studies suggest intact serial position curves among individuals with schizophrenia (83), which is indicative of intact articulatory rehearsal mechanisms.
Working memory is one of the central constructs in cognitive science and has received enormous attention in the theoretical and empirical literature. Similarly, working memory deficits have long been thought to be among the core cognitive deficits in schizophrenia, making it a ripe area for translation. This article provides a brief overview of the current theories and data on the psychological and neural mechanisms involved in working memory, which is a summary of the presentation and discussion on working memory that occurred at the first Cognitive Neuroscience Treatment Research to Improve Cognition in Schizophrenia (CNTRICS) meeting (Washington, D.C.). At this meeting, the consensus was that the constructs of goal maintenance and interference control were the most ready to be pursued as part of a translational cognitive neuroscience effort at future CNTRICS meetings. The constructs of long-term memory reactivation, capacity, and strategic encoding were felt to be of great clinical interest but requiring more basic research. In addition, the group felt that the constructs of maintenance over time and updating in working memory had growing construct validity at the psychological and neural levels but required more research in schizophrenia before these should be considered as targets for a clinical trials setting.
What can research on schizophrenia tell us about the cognitive neuroscience of working memory?
2006, Neuroscience
Work with individuals with lesions to specific brain regions has long been used to test or even generate theories regarding the neural systems that support specific cognitive processes. Work with individuals who have neuropsychiatric disorders that also involve neurobiological disturbances may be able to play a similar role in theory testing and building. For example, schizophrenia is a psychiatric disorder thought to involve a range of neurobiological disturbances. Further, individuals with schizophrenia are known to suffer from deficits in working memory, meaning that examining the work on the neurobiology of working memory deficits in schizophrenia may help to further our understanding of the cognitive neuroscience of working memory. This article discusses the pros and cons of extrapolating from work in schizophrenia to models of healthy working memory function, and reviews the literature on working memory function in schizophrenia in relationship to existing human and non-human primate models of the cognitive neuroscience of working memory.
Neuropsychological Impairments in Schizophrenia: Integration of Performance-Based and Brain Imaging Findings
2007, Psychological Bulletin
(Video) Short Term MemoryDifferential disruptions of working memory components in schizophrenia in an object-location binding task using the suppression paradigm
2006, Journal of the International Neuropsychological Society
Source monitoring: The importance of feature binding at encoding
2008, European Journal of Cognitive Psychology
Recommended articles (6)
Research article
A network analysis of audiovisual affective speech perception
Neuroscience, Volume 256, 2014, pp. 230-241
In this study we were interested in the neural system supporting the audiovisual (AV) integration of emotional expression and emotional prosody. To this end normal participants were exposed to short videos of a computer-animated face voicing emotionally positive or negative words with the appropriate prosody. Facial expression of the face was either neutral or emotionally appropriate. To reveal the neural network involved in affective AV integration, standard univariate analysis of functional magnetic resonance (fMRI) data was followed by a random-effects Granger causality mapping (RFX-GCM). The regions that distinguished emotional from neutral facial expressions in the univariate analysis were taken as seed regions. In trials showing emotional expressions compared to neutral trials univariate analysis showed activation primarily in bilateral amygdala, fusiform gyrus, middle temporal gyrus/superior temporal sulcus and inferior occipital gyrus. When employing either the left amygdala or the right amygdala as a seed region in RFX-GCM we found connectivity with the right hemispheric fusiform gyrus, with the indication that the fusiform gyrus sends information to the Amygdala. These results led to a working model for face perception in general and for AV-affective integration in particular which is an elaborated adaptation of existing models.
Research article
The roles of syllables and phonemes during phonological encoding in Chinese spoken word production: A topographic ERP study
Neuropsychologia, Volume 140, 2020, Article 107382
Languages may differ in their proximate units of phonological encoding in spoken word production. The roles of syllables and phonemes when Mandarin Chinese speakers prepare spoken words remain controversial. In the present study, native Chinese speakers named disyllabic pictures that were preceded by briefly presented and masked prime words while electroencephalogram signals were recorded. Prime words were syllabically or phonemically related to the first syllable of targets or were unrelated. Variables of prime type (syllable overlap or phoneme overlap) and relatedness (related or unrelated) were manipulated in the experiment. Behavioral results demonstrated a critical interaction between prime type and relatedness, with a facilitation effect relative to unrelated primes appearing only in the syllable overlap condition. Spatio-temporal segmentation analysis indicated that syllable relatedness shortened the duration of Map 6 (around 285–410ms), whose time window is associated with the phonological encoding stage. Comparatively, phoneme relatedness shortened the duration of Map 7 (around 410–480ms) in a later time window. Onset latency analysis suggested an earlier onset for the syllable effect (284ms) and a chronologically later onset for the phoneme effect (396ms). Traditional waveform analysis revealed that the syllable overlap condition exerted significant effects on event-related potentials in the time window of 280–440ms, whereas the phoneme overlap condition produced a weak effect in the time window of 400–440ms in a few electrodes. Taken together, these findings substantiate the primacy of abstract syllables in producing Mandarin spoken words and provide initial topographic evidence that Mandarin speakers mentally represent subordinate phonemes at a later stage of word-form encoding.
(Video) Memory: Discovering psychology lecture - Dr Helen Standage (part 1)Research article
Memory recall of traumatic events in refugees
The Lancet, Volume 392, Issue 10160, 2018, p. 2170
Research article
Steps towards understanding the phonological output buffer and its role in the production of numbers, morphemes, and function words
Cortex, Volume 63, 2015, pp. 317-351
The Stimulus Type Effect on Phonological and Semantic errors (STEPS) describes the phenomenon in which a person, following brain damage, produces words with phonological errors (fine→fige), but number words with semantic errors (five→eight). To track the origins of this phenomenon and find out whether it is limited to numbers, we assessed the speech production of six individuals with conduction aphasia following a damage in the left hemisphere, who made phonological errors in words. STEPS was found in all six participants, and was not limited to number words – several other word categories were also produced with semantic rather than phonological errors: function words, English letter names, and morphological affixes were substituted with other words within their category. This supports the building blocks hypothesis: when phonological sequences serve as building blocks in a productive process, they end up having pre-assembled phonological representations, ready for articulation. STEPS reflects a deficit that causes substitutions of one phonological unit with another. In the case of plain content words, this causes substitutions of one phoneme with another, but in the case of pre-assembled phonological units, this causes substitutions of number words with other number words, function words with function words, and morphological affixes with other affixes. An analysis of the participants' functional locus of deficit revealed that they all had a deficit in the phonological output buffer, and this was their only common deficit. We therefore concluded that the pre-assembled phonological units are stored in dedicated mini-stores in the phonological output buffer, which processes not only phonemes but also whole number words, function words, and morphemes. We also found that STEPS depends on the word's role: number words were produced with semantic errors only when they appeared in numeric context, and function words triggered semantic errors only in grammatical context. This suggests that the phonological representation of a word can be obtained either from the phonological output lexicon or from a store of pre-assembled representations in the phonological output buffer, depending on the word's role.
Research article
Neurocognitive and psychological profiles of adult patients with epilepsy in Hong Kong
Epilepsy & Behavior, Volume 29, Issue 2, 2013, pp. 337-343
Cognitive deficits and psychological impairments are often associated with seizures. In order to describe the neuropsychological profiles of adult patients with epilepsy (PWEs) in Hong Kong China, a total of 186 PWEs were recruited with 102 being drug-responsive and 84 being drug-resistant. Symptoms of depression, anxiety, and epilepsy-specific quality of life (QOL) were measured. Cognitive assessments consisted of intelligence, memory, verbal and visual abilities, and executive function. Neurocognitive impairments were prevalent among PWEs, and patients with drug-resistant epilepsy had significantly more impaired psychological and cognitive profiles. Thirty-nine percent and 30% of patients with drug-resistant epilepsy reported moderate to severe levels of anxiety and depression, respectively, versus 16% and 7% of patients with drug-responsive epilepsy. The most commonly occurring cognitive deficit was memory. Thirty-five percent to 47% of patients with drug-resistant epilepsy and 26% to 35% of patients with drug-responsive epilepsy were compromised in verbal recall memory. Our findings also suggested significant correlations between psychological well-being and cognitive performance. Patients who reported more psychological symptoms tended to perform worse in neurocognitive tests. Identification and management of neuropsychological difficulties in PWEs are important and should be included in primary epilepsy care.
Research article
Decoding levels of representation in reading: Arepresentational similarity approach
Cortex, Volume 90, 2017, pp. 88-102
Multiple levels of representation are involved in reading single words: visual representations of letter shape, orthographic representations of letter identity and order, phonological representations of the word's pronunciation, and semantic representations of its meaning. Previous lesion and neuroimaging studies have identified a network of regions recruited during word reading, including ventral occipital-temporal regions and the angular gyrus (AG). However, there is still debate about what information is being represented and processed in these regions. This study has two aims. The first is to help adjudicate between competing hypotheses concerning the role of ventral occipital cortex in reading. The second is to adjudicate between competing hypotheses concerning the role of the AG in reading. Participants read words in the scanner while performing a proper name detection task and we use a multivariate pattern analysis technique for analyzing fMRI data – representational similarity analysis (RSA) – to decode the type of information being represented in these regions based on computationally explicit theories. Distributed patterns of activation in the left ventral occipitotemporal cortex (vOT) and the AGshow evidence of some type of orthographic processing, while the right hemisphere homologues of the vOT supports visual, but not orthographic, information processing of letter strings. In addition, there is evidence of left-lateralized semantic processing in the lvOT and evidence of top-down feedback in the lvOT. Taken together, these results suggest an interactive activation theory of visual word processing in which both the lvOT and lAG are neural loci of an orthographic level of representations.
(Video) Introduction to Cognitive Neuroscience: Session 5.2 (Working memory)
Copyright © 2002 Elsevier Science Ireland Ltd. All rights reserved.
FAQs
What is the phonological similarity effect in short term memory? ›
Traditionally, the phonological similarity effect refers to the finding that immediate serial recall is impaired when lists of items are phonologically similar rather than distinct.
What is phonological similarity in free and serial recall the effect of increasing retention intervals? ›Phonological similarity in free and serial recall: The effect of increasing retention intervals. The phonological similarity effect (or acoustic confusion effect) consists of poor serial recall performance for lists composed of similar‐sounding words compared to lists of dissimilar‐sounding items.
What is phonological short-term memory? ›Phonological short-term memory (PSTM) can be defined as the ability to maintain verbal information in the working memory for a few seconds, that is, to recognize and remember phonological elements and the order of their occurrence.
What is serial recall? ›In a serial recall test, participants are presented with a series of items (such as digits, letters, words, or pictures) and are asked to recall the items in the order in which they were presented, working from the beginning to the end of the series.
What does the phonological similarity effect Tell us about working memory? ›Memory span for a list of phonologically similar words is generally worse than memory span for a list of phonologically dissimilar words, a finding that is called the phonological similarity effect. This finding has often been cited as evidence for the use of phonological coding in short-term memory and working memory.
What is the phonological similarity effect quizlet? ›The phonological similarity effect is the confusion of letters or words that sound similar and R. Conrad (1964) did research on this that showed that if people messed the letters up then they typically said a letter that sounded like the target letter.
What does the phonological similarity effect teach us suggest about our working memory system? ›The phonological similarity effect refers to the finding that similar sounding sets of words are more difficult to retain in memory than sets of phonologically dissimilar words (Conrad & Hull, 1964).
What is the relationship between phonological short-term memory and speech processing? ›Taken together, these results suggest that pSTM is closely connected to the speech production system, even when no spoken output is required. Thus, we have shown that pSTM involves processes overlapping with both speech perception and production.
Is the phonological loop part of short-term memory? ›The phonological loop model advanced by Baddeley (1986), consisting of a short-term store and a subvocal rehearsal process, is the most influential current account of verbal short-term memory.
What does phonological memory affect? ›The ability to hold on to speech-based information in short-term memory is called phonological memory. We rely heavily on our phonological memory when reading and spelling. This skill is assessed by asking students to remember strings of numbers or to repeat nonsense words of increasing length and complexity.